Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Our journals
    • Clinical Medicine
    • Future Healthcare Journal
  • Subject collections
  • About the RCP
  • Contact us

Clinical Medicine Journal

  • ClinMed Home
  • Content
    • Current
    • Ahead of print
    • Archive
  • Author guidance
    • Instructions for authors
    • Submit online
  • About ClinMed
    • Scope
    • Editorial board
    • Policies
    • Information for reviewers
    • Advertising

User menu

  • Log in

Search

  • Advanced search
RCP Journals
Home
  • Log in
  • Home
  • Our journals
    • Clinical Medicine
    • Future Healthcare Journal
  • Subject collections
  • About the RCP
  • Contact us
Advanced

Clinical Medicine Journal

clinmedicine Logo
  • ClinMed Home
  • Content
    • Current
    • Ahead of print
    • Archive
  • Author guidance
    • Instructions for authors
    • Submit online
  • About ClinMed
    • Scope
    • Editorial board
    • Policies
    • Information for reviewers
    • Advertising

Misdiagnosis: analysis based on case record review with proposals aimed to improve diagnostic processes

David Levine and Alan Bleakley
Download PDF
DOI: https://doi.org/10.7861/clinmedicine.11-6-626
Clin Med December 2011
David Levine
Peninsula College of Medicine and Dentistry
Roles: Academic tutor and problem-based-learning facilitator
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Alan Bleakley
Peninsula College of Medicine and Dentistry
Roles: Professor of medical education
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Info & Metrics
Loading

Editor–Neale and colleagues provide a valuable reminder that misdiagnosis is commonly related to cognitive errors (Clin Med August 2011 pp 317–21). They encourage a shift in emphasis from intuitive (system 1) to analytical (system 2) thinking because ‘over-reliance on a simple perceptive approach to diagnosis may forestall analysis’. However, general strategies for correcting cognitive biases can be problematic. Croskerry1 advises against viewing the two systems separately and has promoted a combined approach, emphasising the complexity of decision making in practice. Norman and Eva2 have drawn attention to biases inherent in the analysis of errors. They note that similar errors are attributable to several mechanisms but cite some gains from combining the analytic and intuitive. They suggest that simple prompting strategies may be better than formal teaching about cognitive biases. Thus, to avoid the common bias of premature closure, ‘think of the first thing that comes to mind but think of other possibilities’. Some studies provide evidence that teaching more analytical reasoning may sometimes, paradoxically, worsen results.3 Norman and Eva reference a similar point.2 None of this, of course, negates the need for analytic thought in context.

We have recently proposed that memorable aphorisms can still be valuable aids to judgement.4 While such heuristics (short cuts) have come in for criticism they are not inherently bad (or good) but must be applied in context and reviewed critically. Neale and colleagues seem to be thinking along these lines where they say ‘To remind clinicians not to ignore the pelvis perhaps the term ‘PR (per rectum)’ might be replaced by ‘RPE’ (rectal and pelvic examination)’. Could this be ripe for an aphorism? This important point about terminology influencing behaviour deserves more study. One of us has observed that the ubiquitous use of abbreviations such as ACS or TIA can cause diagnostic error by turning a verbal short cut into thought cut short.5 Few would argue with their suggestion for structural prompts in records. Too often ‘clerking’ is seen as an end in itself. Physicians will support their call for reflection, resisting speed of throughput at the expense of time for thought.

The authors treat the unfolding case as a series of links in a chain, noting error at various points. However, the linear chain is not always a good model for healthcare. Working with patients is a complex system involving uncertainty and unfolding over time. Within such a system, decisions require a wider process of sensemaking and situation awareness that must include networks of persons, as indeed the authors suggest in their recommendation for more consultation. Encouraging doctors (and others) to challenge diagnoses and voice uncertainty is essential, and inculcating these behaviours and collaborative work habits should start in medical school.

Footnotes

  • Please submit letters for the editor's consideration within three weeks of receipt of Clinical Medicine. Letters should ideally be limited to 350 words, and sent by email to: clinicalmedicine{at}rcplondon.ac.uk

  • © 2011 Royal College of Physicians

References

  1. ↵
    1. Croskerry P
    A universal model of diagnostic reasoning Acad Med 2009 84 1022–8doi:10.1097/ACM.0b013e3181ace703
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  2. ↵
    1. Norman GR,
    2. Eva KW
    Diagnostic error and clinical reasoning Med Educ 2010 44 94–100doi:10.1111/j.1365-2923.2009.03507.x
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  3. ↵
    1. Salas E,
    2. Klein GA
    , eds (2001) Linking expertise and naturalistic decision making (Routledge, London).
  4. ↵
    1. Levine DF,
    2. Bleakley A
    (2011) Maximising medicine through aphorisms. Med Educ doi:10.1111/j.1365-2923.2011.04141.x, (in press).
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  5. ↵
    1. Levine DF
    , Personal observations.

Misdiagnosis: analysis based on case record review with proposals aimed to improve diagnostic processes

In response to the thoughtful comments of Levine and Bleakley we are pleased to reemphasise the principal purpose of our paper.

Cognitive shortcuts were first identified as key triggers of errors in judgement and decision taking.1 Subsequently this view was challenged by researchers who have shown the value of ‘cognitively hard-wired’ systems in making sense of complex situations.2 Clearly, ‘intuition’ plays a part in the diagnostic process. However, in our analyses of case records of emergency admissions, we were perturbed to find that the conclusions of clinical clerking by trainees usually appear under the term ‘Imp’ (ie impression). This term is non-specific and as a result follow-up actions may be ill-defined.

Recent evidence demonstrates that ‘getting it right’ using intuition is a marker of expertise. Experienced doctors may be able to arrive at the correct answer very quickly–their expertise has become cognitively implicit. Psychologists have developed a number of techniques to examine this aspect of expertise (including cognitive task analysis and verbal protocol analysis).3 We conclude that intuition-based decision making may work well for an expert diagnostician but is likely to be less fruitful for a trainee physician.

In the light of this evidence we suggest that junior members of the care team should use a systems approach to back up initial thoughts. In caring for older patients, clinicians often face a complex web of possibilities. The proposed simple tabulated format (charting observations, conclusions and resultant actions) allows the team to create a plan for a patient, to share it adequately (including during handovers), and to provide follow-up.

We agree that diagnosis is a non-linear, dynamic cognitive process. It is exactly for this reason that a tabulated plan could help render the process more tractable. Care plans have been shown to improve outcomes in intensive care units.4 However, it is likely that such changes in process can only come from central directives. If it could be shown, in a prospective study, that tabulated observations, conclusions and resultant actions (of the type proposed) significantly enhance the process of assessing acute admissions to hospital, then it would seem reasonable to add such tabulations to the ‘generic medical record-keeping standards’ proposed by the Royal College of Physicians in 2007 and accepted by more than 80% of physicians.5

Footnotes

  • Please submit letters for the editor's consideration within three weeks of receipt of Clinical Medicine. Letters should ideally be limited to 350 words, and sent by email to: clinicalmedicine{at}rcplondon.ac.uk

  • © 2011 Royal College of Physicians

References

  1. ↵
    1. Tversky A,
    2. Kahneman D
    Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases Science 1974 185 1124–31
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    1. Gigerenzer G,
    2. Todd PM
    (1999) ABC Research Group. Simple heuristics that make us smart (Oxford University Press, New York).
  3. ↵
    1. Klein G
    (1999) Sources of power - how people make decisions (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA), doi:10.1061/(ASCE)1532-6748(2001)1:1(21).
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  4. ↵
    1. Pronovost P,
    2. Berenholtz S,
    3. Dorman T,
    4. et al.
    Improving communication in the ICU using daily goals J Crit Care 2003 18 71–5doi:10.1053/jcrc.2003.50008
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    1. Carpenter I,
    2. Bridgelal Ram M,
    3. Croft GP,
    4. Williams JG
    Medical records and record-keeping standards Clin Med 2007 7 328–31
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
Back to top
Previous articleNext article

Article Tools

Download PDF
Article Alerts
Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Citation Tools
Misdiagnosis: analysis based on case record review with proposals aimed to improve diagnostic processes
David Levine, Alan Bleakley
Clinical Medicine Dec 2011, 11 (6) 626-627; DOI: 10.7861/clinmedicine.11-6-626

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero
Share
Misdiagnosis: analysis based on case record review with proposals aimed to improve diagnostic processes
David Levine, Alan Bleakley
Clinical Medicine Dec 2011, 11 (6) 626-627; DOI: 10.7861/clinmedicine.11-6-626
Reddit logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Footnotes
    • References
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Info & Metrics

Related Articles

  • No related articles found.
  • PubMed
  • Google Scholar

Cited By...

  • No citing articles found.
  • Google Scholar

More in this TOC Section

  • Hyperbaric oxygen therapy for the treatment of long COVID
  • Fibromyalgia and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder
  • Same-day emergency care
Show more Letters to the Editor

Similar Articles

FAQs

  • Difficulty logging in.

There is currently no login required to access the journals. Please go to the home page and simply click on the edition that you wish to read. If you are still unable to access the content you require, please let us know through the 'Contact us' page.

  • Can't find the CME questionnaire.

The read-only self-assessment questionnaire (SAQ) can be found after the CME section in each edition of Clinical Medicine. RCP members and fellows (using their login details for the main RCP website) are able to access the full SAQ with answers and are awarded 2 CPD points upon successful (8/10) completion from:  https://cme.rcplondon.ac.uk

Navigate this Journal

  • Journal Home
  • Current Issue
  • Ahead of Print
  • Archive

Related Links

  • ClinMed - Home
  • FHJ - Home
clinmedicine Footer Logo
  • Home
  • Journals
  • Contact us
  • Advertise
HighWire Press, Inc.

Follow Us:

  • Follow HighWire Origins on Twitter
  • Visit HighWire Origins on Facebook

Copyright © 2021 by the Royal College of Physicians